top of page
Writer's pictureGökalp Boz

Understanding Memory: Processes, Distinctions, and Clinical Insights


Memory—a complex cognitive process basic to human experience—pertains to a series of mechanisms that are intricate and fundamentally important for remembering the past, interpreting the present, and planning for the future. Its multifaceted nature is best revealed in the three main phases that can be divided into encoding, storing, and retrieving information. However, this is not a one-piece entity; it is a complex set of sub-processes serving different functions, each reliant on separate neural substrates. This essay goes into the complex cognitive architecture of memory as revealed by clinical observations of memory-impaired patients like H.M. and K.F. From these cases and other similar cases about the distinction between short-term memory and long-term memory and between explicit and implicit memory there is a lot to learn. By taking a critical look at these distinctions and exploring double dissociations between memory processes, an understanding of how memory operates at cognitive, behavioral, and neural levels will be deepened. This introduction sets the stage for a detailed exploration of the complexities of memory and the lessons learned from clinical observations and experimental research.



Illustration of human brain is initiating memory functions as a RAM
Figure 1: Illustration of human brain is initiating memory functions as a RAM (Van Der Wal, 2018).


Memory can be understood as a set of processes crucial in remembering the past, interpreting the present, and planning the future—prospective memory. It involves three basic phases: encoding, storing, and retrieving information. Encoding is the process when, for the first time, you experience information and decide to commit it to memory. Then the storing process involves the maintenance of information that was encoded. Finally, Retrieving is the process by which you access the information that is stored at the time it is needed.


Another thing that is important here to emphasize is that memory and learning are not the same thing. Though closely intertwined, memory and learning are different cognitive processes that each has very distinct functions in the acquisition, retention, and retrieval of information. The differences between these two areas of cognitive thought are fundamental in understanding human cognition. Memory, by definition, is the process of encoding, storing, and retrieving information that was gained in the past. When you look at a picture and immediately know it's one you have seen before, you have retrieved that information from the stored memory, rather than learned something new. Memory is not a singular entity but rather a complex system of various subprocesses, which contribute to different aspects of cognitive function. Based on their cognitive and biological distinctions, such subprocesses comprise the cognitive architecture of memory. Learning, on the other hand, is a process in which a change in behavior occurs due to specific stimuli or training. Unlike memory, which stores and retrieves information, learning actually obtains new knowledge or skills. It is the very act of encoding, which is the process of translating sensory input into stored memory. Whether by classical conditioning, operant conditioning, or observational learning, learning manifests as a change in behavior, knowledge, or understanding in response to environmental stimuli. The interrelationship between memory and learning is complex. While learning initiates the encoding of new information into memory, memory retention is necessary for the consolidation and retrieval of learned material. In addition, memory retrieval can enhance learning by repetition or rehearsal, pushing new knowledge into long-term memory (Kalyuga, 2011).


The first distinction in memory functions divides the processes that are inherently cognitive from those that are behavioral. Information about these distinctions is derived from how these processes are expressed at the biological level and their relationship to behavioral counterparts. The intuition that different processes underpin memory comes from behavioral observations and the idea that different neural structures or processes are responsible for specific cognitive functions. Evidence suggests that memory can be divided into two major sets of sub-processes: short-term or working memory, and long-term memory. Processes of short-term or working memory store information for relatively brief periods (in seconds), while processes of long-term memory store information for extended periods (in years).


The graph of memory types
Figure 2: The graph of memory types (Queensland Brain Institute, n.d.)

Short-term and long-term memory processes are fundamental components of cognitive architecture, providing valuable insights into the mechanisms underlying human memory. Understanding the distinction between these processes not only enhances our comprehension of memory but also underscores the critical role memory functions play in everyday life. Ample evidence from cases of amnesia, often resulting from brain lesions or trauma, illuminates the intricate interplay between short-term and long-term memory systems, shedding light on their distinct functions and neural substrates (Squire & Zola, 1996).


Short-term memory, also known as working memory, is responsible for the temporary storage and manipulation of information needed for immediate tasks. It has a limited capacity and duration, typically retaining information for a few seconds to minutes unless actively rehearsed or transferred to long-term memory. This process allows individuals to hold onto information temporarily, such as remembering a phone number long enough to dial it or following directions to a new location. Short-term memory is crucial for cognitive tasks requiring attention, concentration, and problem-solving, serving as a temporary workspace for mental operations (Squire & Zola-Morgan, 1991; Squire & Zola, 1996).


In contrast, long-term memory involves the encoding, storage, and retrieval of information over extended periods, ranging from minutes to years. It encompasses a vast reservoir of knowledge and experiences accumulated throughout one's lifetime, including facts, events, skills, and semantic concepts. Long-term memory is characterized by its relatively unlimited capacity and enduring retention, allowing individuals to retain information for extended periods, even indefinitely. This process enables the retention of personal experiences, academic learning, and cultural knowledge, shaping individuals' identities and influencing their decision-making and behavior (Squire, 1986; Squire & Zola-Morgan, 1991).


The distinction between short-term and long-term memory processes becomes particularly salient in cases of amnesia, where disruptions to specific memory systems highlight their unique contributions to cognitive functioning. For example, patients with anterograde amnesia exhibit impaired encoding and consolidation of new memories, leading to deficits in forming long-term memories while retaining intact short-term memory abilities. Conversely, individuals with retrograde amnesia may struggle to retrieve past memories while retaining the ability to form new ones, indicative of disruptions to long-term memory consolidation or retrieval processes (Sutherland et al., 2010).



Surrealistic illustration of long-term and short-term memory
Figure 3: Surrealistic illustration of memory (Dalí, 1931).


Amnesia

Amnesia is the temporary or permanent loss of memory. This impairment typically involves loss of long-term memory. Such an impairment can result from a variety of events, including cerebral damage or physical and psychological traumas. Cerebral damage most often is strokes or neural degeneration, while physical traumas, like car accidents, frequently lead to amnesic episodes. Amnesia has a temporal gradient and can be dichotomized into two types. Retrograde Amnesia - These types of amnesia involve the impairment of memories that have already been encoded and stored. Retrograde amnesia is most often caused by psychological or physical trauma. For example, a patient might forget memories of events that happened some hours or even days before a car accident or cerebral injury. Anterograde Amnesia - These types prevent the formation of new memory and are more often associated with cerebral damage. One of the most examined cases of anterograde amnesia is the case of patient H.M. His case significantly advanced the analysis of the cognitive architecture and cerebral correlates of memory function. H.M. had his bilateral hippocampi removed to treat epilepsy, which led to severe anterograde amnesia, thus showing the critical role of the hippocampus in encoding explicit long-term memory (Zhang & Andl, 2022).


Patient H.M. suffered from severe anterograde amnesia and slight retrograde amnesia following the surgical removal of his hippocampi. His condition provided profound insights into the distinct functions of different memory processes. H.M. had cognitive deficits of Severe Anterograde Amnesia. H.M. was incapable of forming new long-term memories. For example, he did not recognize himself in the mirror because he was incapable of updating his body image after the surgery. Another cognitive deficit he showed was Slight Retrograde Amnesia, characterized by difficulty in recalling the events that occurred shortly before his surgery. He would be sad whenever reminded of his brother's death, as he could not retain the memory of this loss.


Despite these impairments, H.M. retained normal short-term memory and implicit memory functions. This dissociation suggested that explicit long-term memory functions are discrete from procedural memory or implicit long-term memory. H.M. could accomplish tasks of short-term memory, including digit-span tasks, but could not transfer this information to long-term storage. The case of H.M. also showed the independence of short-term and long-term memory functions. While he was able to retain information in the short term, he could not encode it into long-term memory; this reflected the different mechanisms and neural substrates that support these kinds of memory (Squire & Wixted, 2011).



MRI arranged of the temporal lobe of patient H.M. (left) to illustrate the removed structures during Patients H.M. ‘s surgery. , PR = Perirhinal cortex, EC = Entorhinal cortex, V = Lateral ventricle, A = Amygdaloid complex, cs = Collateral sulcus
Figure 4: MRI arranged of the temporal lobe of patient H.M. (left) to illustrate the removed structures during Patients H.M. ‘s surgery. , PR = Perirhinal cortex, EC = Entorhinal cortex, V = Lateral ventricle, A = Amygdaloid complex, cs = Collateral sulcus (Squire & Wixted, 2011)

Another significant case in amnesia research was patient KF. Unlike H.M., KF exhibited normal long-term memory performance but impaired short-term memory, particularly in verbal tasks. He could learn word sequences but struggled with single-word span tasks. This case provided crucial evidence that contradicted the idea that storing information for a longer period is inherently more challenging than for a shorter period. It showed that short-term and long-term memory processes are supported by different neural mechanisms. KF's neural damage was distinct from H.M.'s, affecting the lateral part of the hippocampus, specifically in the posterior perisylvian region in the left hemisphere's supramarginal gyrus.


This contrast between patients KF and H.M. underscored the specialized roles of different brain regions in memory processes. While H.M. had difficulties with declarative long-term memory, performing poorly on explicit memory tasks that require conscious recollection, he could perform implicit memory tasks, which involve unconscious memory, relatively well. H.M.'s ability to learn new motor skills, as demonstrated by the mirror drawing task, further highlighted this distinction. In this task, H.M. had to draw a shape while only seeing its reflection, which required training to master. Despite his severe anterograde amnesia, H.M.'s performance improved with practice and was maintained over subsequent days, similar to healthy controls. This indicated that his procedural memory, a type of implicit memory, was intact (Duff et al., 2008).


To fully understand the separation between explicit and implicit memory systems, researchers sought evidence of double dissociation. This would involve identifying another patient who displayed the opposite pattern of deficits—impaired implicit memory but preserved explicit memory. Such findings would solidify the distinction between these two types of memory. For example, motor skill learning impairments are often linked to pathologies affecting the basal ganglia and cerebellum, such as Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease, and cerebellar damage. Additionally, implicit memory can be assessed through tasks like priming. In a priming task, participants are asked to identify words or non-words. Typically, participants respond faster to stimuli they have previously encountered, demonstrating the priming effect (Heindel et al., 1989).


In experiments involving amnesic patients, researchers observed that while these patients showed priming effects—indicating intact implicit memory—they struggled with explicit recognition tasks. For instance, when asked to recognize words presented in a previous lexical decision task, amnesic patients could not recall having seen the words before, even though their implicit memory influenced their faster responses. However, other patients, such as those with lesions in sensory regions, showed no priming effects but had long-term memory performance comparable to healthy controls. This dissociation provided further evidence that explicit and implicit memory systems rely on distinct neural substrates and cognitive processes (Chun & Phelps, 1999; Kantak et al., 2012).


Surrealist illustration of temporal impairments occuring due to amnesia
Figure 5: Surrealist illustration of temporal impairments occuring due to amnesia (Tabitha Smith, 2024)

To sum up, this article explores the major distinctions within the cognitive architecture of memory processes, where clinical observations of memory-impaired patients have contributed vastly. We see how cases such as H.M. and K.F. have been helpful in formulating and testing specific distinctions and double dissociations among these processes. The case of H.M. brought out the distinction between short-term and long-term memory, where anterograde amnesia was almost total but retrograde amnesia was relatively less severe. H.M. could perform tasks requiring short-term memory and implicit memory, such as the mirror drawing task, but was unable to form new long-term declarative memories. Therefore, this case reflected the independence of explicit or declarative and implicit or procedural memory systems. On the other hand, K.F. had impaired short-term memory but intact long-term memory, especially on verbal tasks, due to damage to the posterior perisylvian region. This provided important evidence that short-term and long-term memory are observed by different neural mechanisms. Moreover, we saw that conditions like Parkinson's and Huntington's diseases, which affect the basal ganglia and cerebellum, result in an impairment of implicit motor skill learning, lending further support to the distinction between the explicit and implicit memory systems. In conclusion, clinical observations of patients like H.M. and K.F., besides studies on priming and motor skill learning, have gone a long way in contributing to our knowledge of the complex architecture of memory. These studies reveal the independent yet interlinked nature of short-term and long-term memory processes and point out the distinct roles of explicit and implicit memory systems.



Bibliographical References

Chun, M. M., & Phelps, E. A. (1999). Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with hippocampal damage. Nature Neuroscience, 2(9), 844–847. https://doi.org/10.1038/12222


Duff, M. C., Wszalek, T., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2008). Successful life outcome and management of real-world memory demands despite profound anterograde amnesia. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section A, Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30(8), 931–945. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390801894681


Heindel, W., Salmon, D., Shults, C., Walicke, P., & Butters, N. (1989). Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: a comparison of Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s disease patients. The Journal of Neuroscience, 9(2), 582–587. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.09-02-00582.1989


Kantak, S. S., Mummidisetty, C. K., & Stinear, J. W. (2012). Primary motor and premotor cortex in implicit sequence learning – evidence for competition between implicit and explicit human motor memory systems. European Journal of Neuroscience, 36(5), 2710–2715. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08175.x


Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory: how many types of load does it really need?. Educational Psychology Review, 23(1), 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9150-7


Squire, L. R. (1986). Mechanisms of memory. Science, 232(4758), 1612–1619. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3086978


Squire, L. R., & Wixted, J. T. (2011). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Human Memory Since H.M. In Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34(1), 259–288. Annual Reviews. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113720


Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1996). Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93(24), 13515–13522. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.24.13515


Squire, L. R., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The Medial Temporal Lobe Memory System. Science, 253(5026), 1380–1386. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1896849


Sutherland, R. J., Sparks, F. T., & Lehmann, H. (2010). Hippocampus and retrograde amnesia in the rat model: A modest proposal for the situation of systems consolidation. Neuropsychologia, 48(8), 2357–2369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.015


Zhang, R., & Andl, T. (2022). Brain, memory, and amnesia. Journal of Student Research, 11(3). https://doi.org/10.47611/jsrhs.v11i3.3239

Visual Sources


6 Comments


I appreciate the creativity and attention to detail that went into the creation of poppy playtime, making it a standout experience in the world of indie horror games. Play it and see!

Like

Getting a beautiful, high-profile escort in Delhi is not a big deal. By calling us or visiting our official website, you can easily connect with Aerocity Escorts Service at the Roseate House hotel. It is just the beginning of the happy days, as there is much more enjoyable waiting for you eagerly.

Like

The name of this painting is 'Persistence of Memory'. What do you think the artist wanted to convey?

Like

Nina Mina
Nina Mina
Jun 28

This essay is excellent and really helpful. I've been silently practicing this, and I'm becoming better at it! Enjoy yourself, work harder, and develop your impressiveness free game 2024 candy clicker 2

Like

This improvement was sustained over the following tiny fishing unblocked days, comparable to that of healthy individuals.

Like
Author Photo

Gökalp Boz

Arcadia _ Logo.png

Arcadia has an extensive catalog of articles on everything from literature to science — all available for free! If you liked this article and would like to read more, subscribe below and click the “Read More” button to discover a world of unique content.

Let the posts come to you!

Thanks for submitting!

  • Instagram
  • Twitter
  • LinkedIn
bottom of page